Registration for a live webinar on 'Precision medicine treatment for anticancer drug resistance' is now open.
See webinar detailsWe noted you are experiencing viewing problems
-
Check with your IT department that JWPlatform, JWPlayer and Amazon AWS & CloudFront are not being blocked by your network. The relevant domains are *.jwplatform.com, *.jwpsrv.com, *.jwpcdn.com, jwpltx.com, jwpsrv.a.ssl.fastly.net, *.amazonaws.com and *.cloudfront.net. The relevant ports are 80 and 443.
-
Check the following talk links to see which ones work correctly:
Auto Mode
HTTP Progressive Download Send us your results from the above test links at access@hstalks.com and we will contact you with further advice on troubleshooting your viewing problems. -
No luck yet? More tips for troubleshooting viewing issues
-
Contact HST Support access@hstalks.com
-
Please review our troubleshooting guide for tips and advice on resolving your viewing problems.
-
For additional help, please don't hesitate to contact HST support access@hstalks.com
We hope you have enjoyed this limited-length demo
This is a limited length demo talk; you may
login or
review methods of
obtaining more access.
Printable Handouts
Navigable Slide Index
- Introduction
- The woodpecker’s dilemma
- Environment exploitation
- Solving the patch exploitation problem
- Do models of optimal patch use work?
- Patch exploitation: a ubiquitous problem
- Choice in the laboratory: self-control & impulsivity
- Comparing self-control to patch exploitation
- Implementing patch choice in the lab
- Ecological rationality: travel time effects
- Replication of the patch/self-control difference
- Ecological rationality: decision-making
- Ecological rationality: evolutionary mismatch
- Decision making problems
- Decoy effect & irrelevant options
- Decoy effect: examples
- Decoy effect: sequential decisions
- What choices do animals make?
- Basic principles in decision ecology
Topics Covered
- The patch exploitation problem animal face
- Choice in the laboratory: self-control and impulsivity
- Ecological rationality
- Evolutionary mismatch
- Decoy effect and the independence of irrelevant options
- The importance of comparative preference
Talk Citation
Stephens, D.W. (2017, September 28). Decision ecology: what natural decision-making tells us about the structure of the brain and neural systems [Video file]. In The Biomedical & Life Sciences Collection, Henry Stewart Talks. Retrieved December 23, 2024, from https://doi.org/10.69645/SNCC3076.Export Citation (RIS)
Publication History
Financial Disclosures
- Prof. David W. Stephens has not informed HSTalks of any commercial/financial relationship that it is appropriate to disclose.
Decision ecology: what natural decision-making tells us about the structure of the brain and neural systems
Published on September 28, 2017
35 min
Other Talks in the Series: Ecology
Transcript
Please wait while the transcript is being prepared...
0:00
My name is Dave Stephens.
I'm a professor at the University of Minnesota.
I have a longstanding interest in the evolution of cognitive phenomenon,
sort of learning and decision making.
And today, I want to talk about a topic that I call Decision Ecology.
And in decision ecology,
we're concerned about how decision making systems reflect the natural world,
the world in which animals live.
0:24
And I'd like to start by talking about a specific problem,
which will be kind of the framework around which we'll discuss this problem.
And I want to focus on this particular animal,
this is a pileated woodpecker.
This is a large North American woodpecker,
slightly bigger than a crow.
I don't know if you know how woodpeckers feed,
but we're going to talk about feeding behavior in particular.
But woodpeckers peck wood because they eat the insects that are hidden beneath the bark.
And what they tend to do is fly from tree trunk to tree trunk.
They fly down to the bottom of the tree trunk,
and then they work their way up the tree trunk,
often spiraling up and as they move up the tree trunk,
they are both listening and looking for signs of insect disturbance in the bark of the tree.
And when they see this,
they will dig into it.
They will literally peck the wood.
That's where the name woodpecker comes from.
And they will dig out these insects and consume them.
So it's a pretty amazing phenomenon in many ways.
A sort of, they obviously are sensing
something that humans don't sense when they do this.
They are spectacular animals.
1:29
But I want to talk a little bit about the way they exploit their environment.
To do that, let's think about the kind of world that a woodpecker lives in.
So, this is a little wood lot.
And from our point of view,
it's just a tree, but of course,
this is a food pouch from the point of view of our woodpecker, right?
And not only is it a food pouch but there are many food pouches in this environment,
and if an animal has a problem moving around
these food pouches that I want to draw your attention to.
As it moves in the wood lot,
there are two things it's doing.
One thing is it's moving between trees,
and the other thing is it's actually spending time exploiting trees.
And as I told you, it goes starts at the bottom and works kind of
systematically up exploiting these trees and finding insects within them.
I want you to think a little bit about what the implication of this patchiness is.
And by patchiness, I mean that a woodpecker lives in
a world where there is, you know, it's not a uniform carpet of food,
but food is isolated into different clumps.
And this isn't unique for woodpeckers.
This is a feature of the world that animals live in generally.
And so, when our woodpecker enters a clump of food,
you can imagine that there is kind of
a relationship between the time it spends in its pouch,
or in our case a tree trunk,
and the amount it gains from this food patch.
So what typically happens is that at the beginning of time spent in a patch,
the animal is finding food fairly quickly because the patch is fresh.
It's never been visited before,
but as the animal spends more time in the food patch,
the rate at which it finds new items slows down.
So it has this kind of decreasing marginal rate of gain, right?
We call this relationship between time
spent in the patch and the gain, we call it a gain function, right?
And typically, these gain functions kind of bend down like this, right?
And so this happens because the animal's basically using it up, right?
It's exploiting everything that's there or everything it can find.
Hide